DSpace Repository

Experience and confidence in health technologies: evidence from malaria testing and treatment in Western Kenya

Show simple item record

dc.contributor.author Mangeni, Judith Nekesa
dc.contributor.author Lucy, Abel
dc.contributor.author Taylor, Steve M.
dc.contributor.author Obala, Andrew
dc.contributor.author Prudhomme, Wendy
dc.contributor.author Indrani, Saran
dc.date.accessioned 2022-06-09T06:58:46Z
dc.date.available 2022-06-09T06:58:46Z
dc.date.issued 2022
dc.identifier.uri http://ir.mu.ac.ke:8080/jspui/handle/123456789/6418
dc.description.abstract Background Low adoption of effective health technologies increases illness morbidity and mortality worldwide. In the case of malaria, effective tools such as malaria rapid diagnostic tests (RDTs) and artemisinin- combination therapies (ACTs) are both under-used and used inappropriately. Individuals’ confidence in RDTs and ACTs likely affects the uptake of these tools. Methods In a cohort of 36 households (280 individuals) in Western Kenya observed for 30 months starting in June 2017, we examined if experience with RDTs and ACTs changes people’s beliefs about these technologies and how those beliefs affect treatment behavior. Household members requested a free RDT from the study team any time they suspected a malaria illness, and positive RDT results treated with a free ACT. We conducted annual, monthly, and sick visit surveys to elicit beliefs about the accuracy of malaria RDT results and the effectiveness of ACTs. Beliefs were elicited on a 5-point Likert scale from “very unlikely” to “very likely.” Results Over the study period, the proportion of survey respondents that said a hypothetical negative RDT result was “very likely” to be correct increased from approximately 55–75%. Controlling for initial beliefs, people who had been tested at least once with an RDT in the past year had 3.6 times higher odds (95% CI [1 1.718 7.679], P = 0.001) of saying a negative RDT was “very likely” to be correct. Confidence in testing is associated with treatment behavior: those who believed a negative RDT was “very likely” to be correct had 1.78 times higher odds (95% CI [1.079 2.934], P = 0.024) of adhering to a negative RDT result (by not taking ACTs) than those who were less certain about the accuracy of negative RDTs. Adherence to a negative test also affects subsequent beliefs: controlling for prior beliefs, those who adhered to their previous test result had approximately twice the odds (OR = 2.19, 95% CI [1.661 2.904], P < 0.001) of saying that a hypothetical negative RDT was “very likely” to be correct compared to those who did not. Conclusions Our results suggest that greater experience with RDTs can not only increase people’s confidence in their accuracy but also improve adherence to the test result. en_US
dc.language.iso en en_US
dc.publisher Research Square en_US
dc.subject Health technologies en_US
dc.subject Malaria testing en_US
dc.title Experience and confidence in health technologies: evidence from malaria testing and treatment in Western Kenya en_US
dc.type Article en_US


Files in this item

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record

Search DSpace


Advanced Search

Browse

My Account